Understanding Educational Research
Chris Alexander
Abstract
In this paper I
acknowledge that there is great potential for confusion if the key concepts
adopted by authors do not match the reader’s implicit definition of those
concepts (noted in Mertens 1998,
24). In an effort
to address this problem (i.e. the abuse/misuse of academically ‘respectable’
‘buzz’ words, I will use research terminology with reference to the particular
conceptualisation(s) of specific authors. Crotty (1998, 214) holds that invoking the name of one or other
icon to characterise one’s approach raises interesting questions, one of which
is, why one wants to do that. Even though in this paper I do not assert that
any particular approach is better or worse than any other, I do suggest that as
a wide range of analytical approaches have been developed (especially in the 20th century) it might be worth
considering a ‘Synthetic’ or ‘Maggie’ approach as in the Sim and Loon (2002, 6-7)
sense (i.e. picking and choosing or mixing from a catalogue of theories).
Richard (2000, 934) also takes a similar stance,
she discusses mixing genres (i.e. drawing freely from literary, artistic and
scientific genres often breaking the boundaries of each) arguing the scholar
therefore might have different takes on the same topic. She (ibid) defines this
process as the ‘postmodernist deconstruction of triangulation’. It is held
(Richard 2000, 938) that the ‘blurring’ of
humanities and social sciences is ‘trendy’ because blurring coheres more truly
with the life senses and learning styles of so many. The text I have used in this paper is Gillian
Wigglesworth (2001)
‘Influences on performance in task-based oral assessments’.
Overview
1. Unadulterated
Positivism. 2
2. Wigglesworth’s
research into influences on
performance
in task-based oral assessments. 3
2.1 Research
Questions. 3
2.2 Method and participants. 3
2.3 Instrumentation
and procedure. 3
2.4 Results—major
findings. 4
3. A
discussion of the main weaknesses of the research. 4
4. Researcher
values/attitudes that may have influenced the research. 4-5
5. Suggested
alterations to Wigglesworth’s positivist approach. 6
5.1 Justification
for eliciting more qualitative data. 6
5.2 Suggested
improvements to Wigglesworth’s positivist research. 6
5.3 The
emancipatory paradigm. 7
5.4 The
constructivist paradigm. 7-8
5.5 Introspective
research (my term). 8-9
6. Conclusion. 9
7. References
10-11
Appendix one 11
1. Unadulterated
Positivism
Edgar and Sedgwick
(2002, 203) hold that objectivity in the
natural sciences is an indispensable notion and it presupposes that there is a
real external world which is independent of our knowledge of it i.e. the
concept is that it is possible to describe this world accurately. Richard (2000, 925) notes that given to science is the
belief that its words are objective, precise, unambiguous, non-contextual and
non-metaphoric. Wigglesworth seems to believe in objectivity in the above
senses. She uses (or maybe hides behind) a ‘façade’ of fairly complex
statistical analyses possibly to convince her audience (and maybe also herself)
that objectivity exists, i.e. the world is objective and exists independently
of knowers. The nature of reality (the ontological question) for Wigglesworth
is positivistic in the Mertens (1998,
8-10) sense i.e. one reality exists
and that the researcher’s job is to discover that reality; Guba and Lincoln (1994, 109) call this ‘naïve realism’ i.e. it is
held that ‘research can, in principle, converge on the ‘true’ state of
affairs’. She appears to see research as pure technology or technical process;
she could be a ‘believer’ in the ‘divinity’ of research in the Usher (1996, 14) sense i.e. it is held that there is a
powerful tendency to think of scientific method as universal and ‘made in
heaven’.
Crotty (1998, 26) draws attention to the fact that the
meaning of the term ‘positivism’ has changed and grown over time. Mertens (1998, 7) states that positivism is based on the
rationalistic empiricist philosophy that originated from Aristotle, Frances
Bacon, John Locke, August Comte and Emanuel Kant, though the underlying
assumptions include the belief that the social world can be studied in the same
way as the natural world i.e. value-free methodology and causal-related
explanations. Postpositivists, however, noted in Mertens (1998, 10), recognised certain suppositions
required for scientific research were not appropriate for educational and psychological
research. Heisenberg (quantum theory), noted in Crotty (1998, 29), asserts that the very act of observing
a particle changed that particle. Popper’s Principle of Falsification (1963) noted in Crotty (1998, 31-34)
is another postpositivistic paradigm which recommends that scientists should
make a ‘guess’ and then try to find themselves unable to prove their guess
wrong despite strenuous efforts to do so (noted in Crotty 1998, 31). Feyerabend (1993), cited in Crotty (1998, 37) even asserts that findings are no more
than beliefs and we should not privilege them over other kinds of
beliefs---even voodoo. Mertens (1998,
8-10) notes that in postpositivism a
reality exists but that it can only be known imperfectly because of the
research limitation. Even though Wigglesworth’s assertions are tentative, her
research design does not appear to be post-positivistic; it does not question
the tenants of positivism in a radical fashion as Popper, Feyerabend, or Kuhn
did. It does not have a critical-realism ontology in the Guba and Lincoln sense
(1994, 110) i.e. reality is assumed to
exist but to be only imperfectly apprehendable because of basically flawed
human intellectual mechanisms and the fundamentally intractable nature of the
phenomena.
Wigglesworth’s
epistemology (i.e. what is the nature of knowledge and the relationship between
the knower and would-be knower) is, in my opinion, early positivist i.e. the
researcher and the subject of the study were assumed to be independent and do
not influence each other as in Guba and Lincoln (1994, 110). A postpositivist epistemology would modify
such a belief by recognizing that the theories, hypotheses, and background
knowledge held by the observer may strongly influence the thing that is being
observed (Reichart & Rallis 1994
noted in Mertens 1998,
10). Wigglesworth’s
methodology (i.e. how the knower can go about obtaining the desired knowledge
and understanding) is experimental from the natural sciences; it, for example,
assumes that students can randomly be assigned to conditions (NB this would not
be assumed in postpositivist research noted in Mertens 1998, 10).
Even though Wigglesworth triangulated her data, the emphasis was not
placed on ‘critical multiplism’ i.e. a refurbished version of triangulation as
a way of falsifying rather than verifying hypotheses (noted in Guba and Lincoln
1994, 110).
2 Wigglesworth’s research into influences on
performance in task-based oral assessments.
The paradigm below
summarising the research is partly based on Mertens (1998, 9).
Wigglesworth
undertook her research in AMEP centres. The Adult Migrant Program (AMEP) i.e.
an Australian education and settlement programme for newly arrived migrants and
refugees. Migrants/refugees eligible for teaching receive up to a maximum of 510 hours to enable them to
achieve functional English (NB students also learn about the Australian way of
life).
2.1 Research
Questions
This study was an
attempt to report on the kind of impact pre-determined (i.e. by the author)
combinations of 5
task variables would have on learner performance in relatively informal
classroom based assessment. The variables were: (1) cognitive difficulty of task; (2) whether the interlocutor was a
native or non-native speaker; (3)
if planning time was made available; (4)
task familiarity; (5)
structure. The author (2001,
186) justifies her
approach by arguing that the majority of work on task variability and its
effect on learner language was focused on classroom-based tasks, with few
studies examining tasks in the context of assessment.
2.2 Method and participants
80 learners (i.e. AMEP migrants and refugee) from
various ESL centres took part in the research project. Five routinely-used,
competency-based, AMEP-teacher-developed, evaluative tasks at vocational and
proficiency levels were identified. There were four variable-manipulated tasks (i.e. taken by randomly-assigned groups
of 20 learners),
and a control task, which was thought to be ‘universally-familiar’ to every
student, for all 80
learners to take. Trained and experienced teachers administered the tasks.
Student feedback on task difficulty was sought by using a five-point Likert
scale. The interviewers were thought to be familiar with the rating scales and
performances were randomly double-rated.
2.3 Instrumentation
and procedure
Three discrete
quantitative calculations were made to ascertain the difficulty level of each
task: (1) an
analysis of variance and T-tests on the rater raw scores measured subject
performance; (2) A
Rasch analysis for four facets was undertaken using the statistical modelling
program FACETS; (3)
learner feedback regarding task difficulty was elicited.
2.4 Results—major
findings
·
Tasks seemed easier when the interlocutor was a
NNS. Wigglesworth (2001,
204) believes
three reasons might explain how NS-interlocutors might make a task more
difficult: (1)
learners may be more relaxed with NNS; (2)
raters may compensate for a perceived disadvantage in having a NNS
interlocutor; (3)
learners’ language might be less complex with NNS interlocutors
·
Planning time appeared to adversely affect
performance in both structured and unstructured tasks possibly because planning
time encourages learners to attempt (i.e. on the whole unsuccessfully) to use
more complicated language.
3. A
discussion of the main weaknesses of the research
Even though this is
an ambitious research project with impressive data analyses, reducing the
number of variables and considering/explaining more carefully how these
variables should be grouped would have made the data analysis more
straightforward. The following points may affect the reliability and validity
of the findings.
·
The data are held (2001, 186) to be pertinent to informal classroom
assessment, though the method of assessment seemed formal. It was not clear how
this test was perceived by the participants: did they take it as seriously as a
formal high-stakes examination?
·
Wigglesworth did not explain why other learners
were chosen to be NNS interlocutors and on what grounds they were chosen.
·
Wigglesworth did not consider how physical
characteristics, psychological factors and experiential characteristics played
affected these results.
·
Please refer to appendix one for more
weaknesses regarding the data analysis.
Even though this
study could be replicated and may have had some impact on AMEP policy and
practice, the positivist research philosophy adopted has implications for the
data that was elicited. In section 3,
I have drawn attention to some weaknesses in Wigglesworth’s ‘positivist’ methodology;
I however do hold a ‘positivist’
contribution to knowledge has been made.
4. Researcher
values/attitudes that may have influenced the research
In section four I
will discuss two issues that may have affected this research. The first, is,
how was this research funded and why? The second point pertains to the fact
that Wigglesworth did not consider the stress migrant/refugee students may
experience in such language programmes.
Young et al (2002, 215)
hold that a new mood characterises the funding and execution of social
research i.e. it has taken a ‘utilitarian turn’, a return to an expectation
that social science should be useful. Usher (1996, 29-32) warns it is not enough to adopt
the unquestioning position that research is simply disinterested pursuit of
truth. He further states (ibid) that placing stress on the seeking of a ‘truth
out of many truths’ fails to consider the workings of power within the research
process. He poses (1996,
32) a reflexive
question i.e. what is this research silent about? Race, gender, class
relationships?
Wigglesworth (2001) did not mention that she
was affiliated with AMEP in her paper; she only briefly mentions (p. 206) in a footnote section that the research was
carried out as part of an NCELTR special project funded by the Department of
Immigration and Multicultural Affairs. An Internet search, (http://www.nceltr.mq.edu.au/)
revealed that NCELTR stands for the Centre for English Language at Macquarie University. On her Internet home-page
biography site, (http://www.linguistics.unimelb.edu.au/contact/staff/wigglesworth.html),
however, Wigglesworth mentions she had been working at Macquarie University
between 1995 and 2001 and also at the Adult
Migrant English Program Research Centre (i.e. AMEP) from 2000-2001. Why did the funding for the project go
to Macquarie University and not AMEP? Was this
disinterested research? Was the ‘truth’ that Wigglesworth ‘chose’ to discover
biased by her affiliation with AMEP? Did/does the Department of Immigration and
Multicultural Affairs have an agenda with regard to migrants or refugees? On the
following Australian public-document page http://unpan1.un.org/intradoc/groups/public/documents/un/unpan000211.html
(section E) for example, it is stated that
‘the immigration
policy debate is shifting inextricably towards economic considerations, which
means that as unemployment is likely to remain unacceptably high and the
immigration policy domain will become correspondingly important’.
Did research funders expect the ‘truth’ to be
relevant to ‘their’ agenda? How would for example Wigglesworth’s, in my
opinion, ‘debateable’ ‘planning time’ finding (i.e. giving students less
planning appeared to make tasks easier) fit into this possible ‘agenda”? David (2002, 213-214) states that in the last
decade especially in Australia,
the notions of ‘evidence’ and social science research have often been elided
with political movements for social and political change. He also maintains
(ibid) that there have been many contradictory approaches to what counts as
evidence and how it may be used to modernise governance and build public
policies for the future.
Edgar and Sedgwick
(2002, 1) hold that cultural studies have
sought to draw attention to issues of race, class and gender. Why Wigglesworth,
a white female researcher, did not consider the need to address such issues in
her research is interesting; did she feel such data would not be of interest to
the funders or was she not interested in this herself? According to Marxist
theory the dominant class (noted in Edgar and Sedgwick 2002, 47) in a capitalist society (the
bourgeoisie) is strictly in opposition to the systematically exploited
proletariat (i.e. those that have to sell their ability to work). I however
feel that in this ‘globalised’ ‘multi-nationalised’ world there may actually be
two extremes in a continuum, migrant/refugee labourers clearly being at the
lower ‘proletariat’ end of the continuum. Wigglesworth in her research did not consider
the more qualitative issues appertaining to how threatening AMEP migrant/refugee
students might perceive such a test or the programme in general i.e. being an
ethnic minority underclass to be mainly employed in low-income service-related
occupations with little hope of breaking out of the resulting and unavoidable
vicious cycle of poverty that sadly has become synonymous with such work. Why
did Wigglesworth not consider the stress such migrant students might be going
through while undergoing the AMEP ‘socialisation’ process (i.e. the process
whereby the individual learns to be a member of a particular society or culture
noted in Edgar and Sedgwick 362)?
Could such stress or worry not be considered valid variables in task-based AMEP
learning and so worthy of analysis and relevant to the funders? With regard to
ethical research issues, Wigglesworth did not mention whether the participants
had agreed to participate in the study through voluntary consent forms (i.e.
without undue threat or inducement) as Mertens (1998, 24) recommends.
5.
Suggested alterations to Wigglesworth’s positivist approach.
In this section I will: (1)
draw attention to some problems concerning quantification and present
some persuasive arguments for eliciting qualitative data as well; (2) propose a synthetic approach
based on different inquiry paradigms. The advantages and disadvantages of
paradigms will be
considered.
5.1
Justification for eliciting more qualitative data.
Guba and Lincoln (1994, 106-107)
note that in recent years strong counter pressures against quantification have
emerged. The term (ibid) ‘context striping’ is used to refer to the issue of
not considering other variables that may, if allowed to exist, change the
research findings. Since such exclusionary designs have implications for the
applicability/generalisability of the research, it is argued (ibid) that
eliciting more qualitative data can redress the imbalance by providing
contextual information and rich insights into human behaviour (e.g. the ‘emic’
view). Padilla and Lindholm (1995)
noted in Mertens (1998,
24) maintain that
the nomothetic approach seeks confirmation of general laws and uses procedures
that parallel the natural sciences, in contrast, the idiographic approach seeks
to uncover a particular event in nature or society; idiographic data would be
more qualitative and particularistic. Guba and Lincoln (1994, 106-107)
hold that quantitative normative methodology, i.e. ‘science’, is privileged
over the insights of creative and divergent thinkers and assert (ibid) that
qualitative inputs may redress the imbalance; it is also noted (ibid) that some
critics of the ‘received view’ suggest using alternative paradigms that involve
not only qualitative methodology but fundamental adjustments in the basic
assumptions that guide the research. In light of the above, it is suggested
that more qualitative data be elicited within the context of an inquiry
paradigm shift (i.e. the metaphysics or basics beliefs change) and it is also
held that since there appears to be no ‘one truth’ that human beings can
readily apprehend, a combination of inquiries (i.e. a synthetic approach NB see
the abstract) may provide a variety of ‘takes’ all of which could be relevant
to this research project, and maybe even to its funders. I therefore maintain
that the positivist research undertaken by Wigglesworth should only be one of
the ‘ingredients’ of the ‘research mixture’ or parts in such a ‘synthesis’.
5.2 Suggested
improvements to Wigglesworth’s positivist research.
In section three, I
drew attention to a number of weaknesses in Wigglesworth’s research design and
data analysis. My main recommendations were to reduce the number of research
variables and to consider more carefully the data analysis. However considering
a post positivist design instead of/or in addition to the positivist, as in the
Guba and Lincoln (1994,
110) sense, with a
critical ontology might provide an interesting perspective on the reality being
observed. The epistemology (modified dualist/objectivist) as in Guba and
Lincoln (ibid) would abandon dualism as not possible to maintain; replicated
findings would probably be true but would be subject to falsification. The
methodology (modified experimental) would emphasise ‘critical multiplism’ as a
way of falsifying rather than verifying (as in the Guba and Lincoln sense ibid). However, whether the funders would be
interested in knowing that a ‘certain’ hypothesis had been falsified as opposed
to being informed about ‘hard
positivist-research facts’ is not clear.
5.3 The
emancipatory paradigm
In section four, I
argued that Wigglesworth did not consider race, class, gender as variables and
that there were strong arguments for eliciting qualitative data. Other research
paradigms however may be more suited to eliciting qualitative data (e.g.
constructivist noted in Guba and Lincoln 1994, 112-113 or the emancipatory noted in
Mertens 1998, 8).
As cultural relativism holds that there are different standards of
morality, practices and belief systems which operate in different cultures and
cannot be judged with regard to their worth from a standpoint exterior to them
(noted in Edgar and Sedgwick, 2002,
99), an
emancipatory paradigm in the Mertens’ sense (ibid), might be relevant to making
research more relevant to race, class and gender.
Mertens (1998, 15-19)
maintains that the emancipatory paradigm includes critical theorists,
participatory action researchers, ethnic minorities among others and is
contrasted with Guba and Lincoln’s
(1994) critical
theory ‘label’. It is held in Mertens (ibid) that the emancipatory paradigm
deals directly with the politics in research by confronting social oppression
wherever it occurs. One justification for the emancipatory paradigm concerns
the realisation that much of sociological and psychological theory was
developed from white, able-bodied male perspective and was based on the study
of male subjects (noted in Mertens ibid). Was Wigglesworth’s research race
sensitive? How many of the participants were males/females? What were their
ethnic minority backgrounds? Was there
any correlation between gender, race, (class) and achievement in the programme?
How did teacher expectations vary depending on the ethnicity, gender, class,
sexuality or disability of the students? Was there an asymmetric power
relationship between the student and school staff? If so, how did this affect
motivation? The above are some introductory emancipatory questions that could
become the basis of a different research perspective.
Emancipatory
ontology would recognise multiple realities though would critically examine
findings via an ideological critique in terms of its role in perpetuating
oppressive social structures and policies (noted in Mertens ibid). Therefore,
Wigglesworth could also have looked at the way social, political, cultural,
economic, ethnic and gender values effected/ affected the programmes. Mertens
for example holds (1998,
20-21) that in the case of Oakes and
Guiton’s (1995)
study, ‘race, ethnicity and social class were used as a basis for signifying a
student’s ability and motivation, thus influencing curriculum decisions’. The epistemology of the emancipatory approach
would be interactive and empowering to those without power. Mertens (ibid)
maintains starting off thought by considering the marginalised; this would
expose some of the unexamined researcher assumptions and generate more critical
questions (e.g. considering ways research benefits or does not benefit the
participants). An emancipatory methodology could combine quantitative and
qualitative methods of data collection (noted in Mertens ibid) and provide a
valuable alternative perspective on issues relevant to AMEP, however funders
may not ‘feel happy’ about funding such potentially long term and
‘controversial’ research.
5.4 The
constructivist paradigm
The constructivist
paradigm was developed from the philosophy of Edmund Hessel’s phenomenology and
Wilhelm Dilthey’s and other German philosophers’ study of interpretive
understanding called hermeneutics (noted in Mertens 1998 ,11). Mertens (ibid) states that the basic
underlying assumptions of the interpretative/constructivist paradigm are that
knowledge is socially constructed by people active in the process of research
i.e. the emphasis is that research is affected by the values of the researcher
and cannot be independent of them. The ontology (according to Mertens 1998, 11-12)
therefore assumes that multiple, and sometimes conflicting, mental
constructions can be apprehended i.e. the goal therefore is to understand
multiple social constructions of meaning. The epistemology would (noted in
Mertens 1998, 13) suppose the inquirer and
inquired-into were interlocked in an interactive process, each influencing the
other. A constructivist methodology would require qualitative methods of data
elicitation such as interviews, observations and document reviews. It is held
(Mertens 1998, 14) that reality should be
constructed on the basis of the interpretation of data with the help of the
participants who provide the data NB certain questions cannot be formulated
before the study begins.
Wigglesworth could
therefore attempt to elicit data from the programme participants regarding
aspects of programme, and then begin to formulate some research questions. She
would have to provide data regarding the backgrounds of the participants and
the context in which they were being studied. The assumption is that it is the
interpretation of the multiple participant perceptions of ‘reality’ that
distinguishes this paradigm from the two mentioned in sections 5.2 and 5.3 above. The findings could be of interest
to the funders, though this kind of research:
(1) takes
longer to complete; (2)
could be more disruptive to the programme in general; (3) would be more expensive to undertake as
it would involve several visits to programme centres around Australia.
5.5 Introspective
research (my term)
Richard recommends
(2000, 934-938) ‘breaking boundaries’ by considering
evocative forms i.e. a blurring of humanities with social sciences can be
achieved and should be welcomed by drawing freely from literary, artistic and
scientific genre. I believe Wigglesworth could also provide some introspective
data about herself in the form of diary entries, essays or even poems. The idea
would be to ‘try’ to understand herself better (i.e. not only as a researcher)
possibly by writing about the following:
(1)
Edgar and Sedgwick (2002, 122) argue that a patriarchal culture privileges a
hierarchical way of thinking grounded in a series of oppositions e.g. such as
male/female, intelligible/sensitive, active/passive. Sim and Loon (2002, 90-91)
use the term binary opposition and hold that discourse in the West in general
is based on such opposition; man/woman would be such an opposition. Even though
there are many dedicated female researchers in the area of language testing, the
degree to which they feel they have to compete with male researchers is not
clear. I feel Wigglesworth might consider writing an essay about how it feels
to be a (white) female researcher in a patriarchal culture; Richard (2002, 932) notes that graduate students have found
the idea of the writing-story (i.e. writing about themselves, their work
spaces, disciplines, friends, and families) useful for thinking through and
writing about their research experiences. It is held (ibid) that some
researchers ‘use the writing-stories as an alternative or supplement to
traditional methods chapter and as a link to narratives of those they have
researched’.
(2)
What drove her to research the things she
researched? Wigglesworth could write an account (e.g. an essay or diary account)
of what, for example, she ‘really’ felt of the funders and/or whether she
thought they had a ‘hidden agenda’. It could also attempt to delve into her
innermost concerns as a researcher and/or her desire to acquire public
recognition as a researcher. I believe there is not enough of this kind of very
personalised qualitative ‘introspective’ feedback accompanying positivistic or
post-positivistic research.
These two ‘unusual’
introspective suggestions may provide some extremely interesting supplementation
to ‘traditional’ research. Even though following up these suggestions may help
Wigglesworth clarify certain issues, presenting such ‘data’ publicly may have
unforeseen consequences for the researcher.
6. Conclusion
In this paper I
have suggested that mixing an improved version of Wigglesworth’s positivist
research with post-positivist, emancipatory, constructivist and introspective
(my term) approaches will provide different perspectives and therefore ‘takes’
on ‘our’ intangible reality (i.e. reality appears to be intangible for humans).
With regard to
‘our’ intangible reality, even though life experience and cultural background
certainly affect the way we interpret ‘social situations’, the way we interpret
‘things/objects’ may actually, in part, be preconditioned by evolution i.e. our
minds—the black box—receive complex data through our ‘imperfect’ and ‘limited’
senses; data is then interpreted almost instantaneously using ‘algorithms’
unknown to us. If our senses were more acute and if the algorithms were
‘better’ maybe ‘reality’ would be quite different, maybe there would only be
one reality.
7. References
Crotty,M. (1998). The Foundation of Social Research. London: Sage Publications
David, M. (2002). ‘Themed Section on
Evidence-Based Policy as a Concept for Modernising
Governance
and Social Science Research’. Social
Policy or Society. 1:3, 213-214
Edgar, A and
Sedgewick, P. (2002).
Cultural Theory, the Key Concepts. London. Routledge
Feyerabend, P. (1993). Against Method. London:
Verso
Guba, E. and
Lincoln, Y. (1994).
Competing paradigms in qualitative research. In N. K. Denzin
& Y.
S. Lincoln (Eds.), The handbook of
qualitative research (pp. 105-117). Thousand Oaks, CA:
Sage
Mertens, D. (1998). Research methods in education and Psychology. London. Sage Publications
Oakes, J., &
Guiton, G. (1995).
Matchmaking: The dynamics of high school tracking
decisions
. American Educational Research Journal, 32(1), 3-33.
Padilla, A. M.,
& Lindholm, K. S. (1995).
Qualitative educational research with ethnic
minorities.
In J. A. Banks & C. A. McGee-Banks (Eds.), Handbook of research on multicultural education (pp 97-113) New
York: Macmillan
Popper, K. R. (1963). The Logic of Scientific discovery. New York: Basic Books.
Reichardt, C. S.
& Rallis, S. F. (1994),
Qualitative and quantitative inquiries are not
incompatible:
A call for a new partnership. In C. S. Reichardt & S. F. Rallis (eds), The qualitative/quantitative debate (New
Directions for Program Evaluation, Vol 61
pp 85-91). San Francisco, CA:
Jossey-Bass
Richards, L. (2000). A Method of Inquiry. In N.
K. Denzin & Y. S. Lincoln (Eds), The
Handbook of qualitative research (pp 923-948). Thousand
Oaks, CA: Sage
Sim, S., &
Loon, B, V (2002).
Introducing critical theory. Cambridge: Icon Books UK.
Usher, R. (1996). A critique of the
neglected epistemological assumptions of education
research.
In Scott, D. and Usher, R. Understanding
Educational Research. London:
Routledge
Wigglesworth (2001). ‘ Influences on
performance in task-based oral assessment’. In Bygate, M.,
P.
Skehan and M. Swain. (eds) 2001.
Researching pedagogic tasks: second language learning, teaching and
testing. New York:
Pearson Education.
Young, K., Ashby,
D., Boaz, A., and Grayson, L. (2002).
‘ Social Science and the Evidence-
based
Policy Movement’. Social Policy and
Society. 1:3, 215-224
Appendix one
Some additional weaknesses in Wigglesworth’s research
·
Wigglesworth did not explain what an
‘unmanipulated’ task was. Furthermore it was stated (p194) that there were no significant
differences for learners participating at either level in these baseline tasks
(i.e. in the non manipulated tasks). I find this result surprising as it would
suggest that the students were all at the same language level (which is unusual
at any ESL centre), yet Wigglesworth did not mention that the learners were
same-level learners or on what basis they were chosen; she only stated (p191-192) that they ‘were drawn from different
ESL centres. An indication of the level of tasks was given on (p206) If they were drawn randomly how was it
possible that they were all at the same linguistic level? I suppose, at least
for the purposes of the research, it would be important to ‘ensure’ that
there were no differences in the control-task data, as ‘differences’ at this
stage in the research would raise
questions about the usefulness/relevance of the rest the research data. I think
presenting all the data for tasks one would have been of interest.
·
There
were two level types: functional level of proficiency (tasks 1-3) and vocational level of proficiency (tasks 4-5). Yet the use of these terms were not defined or
justified in detail and how tasks were level-grouped seemed debatable. For
instance (P195)
giving instructions about how to use a bank automatic teller machine was seen
as functional, yet explaining to a 12-year-old
child how to change a light bulb was seen as vocational (and not
functional).
·
With regard to the five-point Likert scale for
student feedback, even though (p206)
some categories had been ‘collapsed’ with others, I felt the ‘easy’ and ‘OK.’
categories presented in the data analyses were vague; what is the semantic
difference between ‘easy’ and ‘OK’ and would a learner not interpret these
words a slightly synonymous?
·
With regard to task type one (p 195) it was stated that the more
familiar task without planning was the easiest i.e. task 3, yet in my opinion student feedback
indicated that task 2
(familiar + planning) was the easiest.
·
In task type three the student feedback partly
contradicts (i.e. more students find this easy) Wigglesworth’s finding
regarding the NNS interlocutor making the task easier